
Unraveling Cognitive Aging: A Comprehensive Narrative Review of the Seattle Longitudinal Study and Recent Breakthroughs
Public HealthReceived 17 Jul 2025 Accepted 20 Aug 2025 Published online 21 Aug 2025
ISSN: 2995-8067 | Quick Google Scholar
Next Full Text
Scientific Productivity of the Internet of Things (IoT) in 2011-2025: A Bibliometric Analysis
Received 17 Jul 2025 Accepted 20 Aug 2025 Published online 21 Aug 2025
The Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS), initiated in 1956 by Dr. K. Warner Schaie, is one of the most comprehensive psychological research studies on cognitive aging. It aims to investigate various aspects of psychological development throughout adulthood, defined within the age range of 22 to 70. It focuses on individual differences and differential patterns of change for selected psychometric abilities from young adulthood to midlife to old age. It has determined the magnitude and relative importance of age changes in various cohorts in different skills [
].9The study began with a random sample of 500 adults in Seattle, with their ages ranging from their 20s to their 60s. The study has continued in seven-year intervals since 1956, with each interval inviting an average of 375 new participants (selected from the Group Health Cooperative membership with 400,000 members) [
]. In addition to the main study, siblings of main study participants also participated in studies to determine the extent of family similarity in mental abilities and other psychological characteristics [ ]. The addition of grandchildren to the primary survey in 2002 made SLS the first three-generation study of cognitive abilities ever conducted in the United States.Despite the passing of Dr. Schaie in 2015, the SLS study continues in his legacy. The cohort-sequential longitudinal study, which examines cognitive and psychosocial change in multiple birth cohorts over the same chronological age, is unique and informative to researchers and the general public. Its findings have been used in legal proceedings on age discrimination in employment and policy discussion regarding mandatory retirement practices in the United States and Canada [
].This narrative review aims to (a) summarize key findings from the Seattle Longitudinal Study, including determining to what degree it answered its five key objectives: 'whether intelligence changes uniformly throughout adulthood or if there are different life-course-ability patterns; at what age and at what magnitude decrement in ability can be reliably detected; the patterns of individual differences in cognitive aging; the determinants of individual differences in cognitive aging; and whether educational interventions can reverse intellectual decline' [
]. Additionally, this review aims to (b) examine its impact on the theories of brain development and cognitive aging, (c) identify opposing or conflicting viewpoints from contemporary studies, and (d) suggest possible loopholes or distinct fields worthy of further research. The literature review will then be followed by a more comprehensive systematic review answering the following research question: "How have the findings of the Seattle Longitudinal Study been supported or challenged by subsequent research?".'The Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS; Hertzog, 2010; Schaie, 1996a, b, 2000, 2005a) began as Schaie's doctoral dissertation at the University of Washington (Seattle, WA) in 1956' [
]. It has then continued for more than 60 years, with the database consisting of data collected from 7 primary testing cycles: 1956, 1963, 1970, 1977, 1984, 1991, 1998 [ ]. It has then continued for more than 60 years, with the database consisting of data collected from 7 primary testing cycles: 1956, 1963, 1970, 1977, 1984, 1991, 1998 [ ]. However, it is more than just one three-generational longitudinal study, instead also consisting of side studies consisting of parent-offspring and sibling pairs, as well as short-term cross-sectional studies [ ].All initial study participants are initially members of the Health Maintenance Organization - the Group Health Cooperative of Puget Sound, in the Seattle, Washington metropolitan area [
]. The initial sampling size consists of 18000 potential adult participants, stratified based on age and sex, with 25 men and 25 women randomly selected for each year of birth from 1889 to 1939.' The study acknowledges the consistent attrition rates from the passing away of older people, implementing measures such as (a) retesting survivors from previous studies, and (b) sampling those untested in previous studies from the Group Health Cooperative.The longitudinal design of the Seattle Longitudinal Study with seven primary testing cycles is known as the cohort-sequential design, an approach that satisfactorily combines the cross-sectional, longitudinal, and sequential components [
]. It allows researchers to disentangle age, cohort, and period effects, providing both short-term and long-term developmental data. In other words, it enables the examination of individual changes and differences between people influenced by contemporary factors over time [ ]. As introduced previously, the SLS utilized a 'sampling with replacement' approach in each new wave, retesting surviving participants from previous waves and adding new randomly selected participants, maintaining the sample size and age distribution over time.The Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) assesses five primary cognitive abilities: verbal comprehension, spatial orientation, inductive reasoning, numeric ability, and perceptual speed. These abilities are crucial for understanding cognitive aging and development. The tests used in SLS include the Primary Mental Abilities (PMA) test battery, which measures verbal meaning, spatial orientation, inductive reasoning, numeric ability, and word fluency. Over time, additional tests such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) have been incorporated to provide a more comprehensive assessment. To keep up with advancements in the cognitive psychology field, the cognitive battery has evolved to include tests addressing memory and executive function [
].The Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) included physical health measurements encompassing evaluations of chronic illnesses, assessments of functional abilities, cognitive health screenings for conditions like dementia, and measurements of Body Mass Index (BMI) and other anthropometric indicators. It also featured comprehensive health questionnaires that covered medical history, family health backgrounds, and lifestyle factors such as dietary habits, physical activity, and substance use. Environmental factors were also assessed, including socioeconomic status, living conditions, and social engagement. Finally, genetic data collection involved gathering DNA samples, identifying genetic markers, and compiling family history pedigrees.
The researchers of the Seattle Longitudinal Study conducted testing sessions at seven-year intervals since the study began in 1956, with each typically lasting for several hours, where the participants were subjected to various cognitive and psychological assessments as detailed above. The sessions were conducted in a controlled environment, often within the facilities of the Group Health Cooperative in Washington State. Research assistants underwent rigorous training from geropsychology experts to ensure consistency and accuracy in data collection. Further, quality control measures included regular supervision, calibration of testing instruments, and periodic reviews of data collection procedures. Finally, missing data were addressed using statistical techniques such as multiple imputation and maximum likelihood estimation to ensure the robustness of the findings.
The Seattle Longitudinal Study used various statistical methods, including growth curve and structural equation modeling, to analyze the data. This method allowed researchers to examine changes in cognitive abilities over time and identify factors influencing these changes. Meanwhile, longitudinal data were analyzed using hierarchical linear modeling and latent growth curve analysis techniques to separate age, cohort, and time-of-measurement effects. These analyses helped to disentangle the effects of aging from those of cohort differences and time-specific influences.
To properly address ethical considerations, informed consent was obtained from all participants at each data collection wave. The participants were provided detailed information about the study's purpose, procedures, and potential risks and benefits. To protect participant confidentiality, code identifiers and secure data storage are used, with access restricted to authorized personnel only. In summary, the study adhered to ethical guidelines set forth by institutional review boards (IRBs) and followed protocols to ensure the ethical treatment of participants.
After the age of 60, a gradual decline in cognitive abilities becomes increasingly apparent, though the rate and extent of this decline vary significantly among individuals [
]. The SLS has consistently demonstrated that while most individuals experience some degree of cognitive slowing or memory impairment after 60, the pace of deterioration differs based on factors such as baseline cognitive functioning, lifestyle choices, and overall health status [ ]. Some individuals maintain high levels of cognitive performance well into their seventies, while others show more pronounced deficits earlier. This variability highlights the need for personalized approaches to understanding and addressing cognitive aging [ ]. Furthermore, longitudinal analyses from the SLS indicate that the decline in cognitive function is not necessarily linear; rather, it may occur in spurts or follow a more gradual trajectory depending on individual circumstances [ ]. These insights have important implications for both clinical practice and public health initiatives aimed at promoting cognitive resilience in aging populations [ ].Several modifiable factors have been identified as contributors to better cognitive outcomes, particularly education, occupation, and participation in intellectually stimulating activities [
]. Educational attainment appears to play a crucial role in cognitive resilience, as individuals with higher levels of education tend to perform better on cognitive assessments and show less decline over time [ ]. Similarly, occupational complexity-particularly jobs requiring problem-solving, decision-making, and interpersonal communication associated with sustained cognitive function in later life [ ]. Engaging in mentally challenging leisure activities, such as puzzles, games, or learning new skills, also contributes to maintaining cognitive abilities. These findings suggest that cognitive stimulation throughout the lifespan can serve as a protective factor a Decline. 17 As such, promoting lifelong intellectual engagement may be an effective strategy for enhancing cognitive health in aging populations [ ].These cohort differences highlight the influence of historical and environmental contexts on cognitive aging [
]. Individuals born in the early 20th century experienced different developmental conditions-such as limited educational opportunities, economic hardships, and inadequate healthcare-compared to those born in the latter half of the century. As a result, later-born cohorts tend to have higher baseline cognitive abilities and experience slower rates of cognitive decline. The SLS findings underscore the dynamic nature of cognitive aging and emphasize the need to consider generational shifts when interpreting longitudinal data on cognitive change [ ]. Understanding these cohort effects is essential for developing accurate models of cognitive aging and tailoring interventions to support cognitive health across diverse populations.In addition to physical health, personality traits also exert an influence on cognitive aging, with certain characteristics being modestly correlated with cognitive abilities [
]. The SLS has explored the relationship between personality and cognition, revealing that traits such as conscientiousness, openness to experience, and emotional stability are associated with better cognitive outcomes in older adults [ ]. Conscientious individuals, who tend to be organized, disciplined, and goal-oriented, often exhibit stronger cognitive performance and slower rates of decline. Similarly, those who score high on openness to experience-characterized by curiosity, creativity, and a willingness to learn-demonstrate greater cognitive flexibility and adaptability in later life [ ]. Emotional stability, which reflects an individual's ability to manage stress and maintain psychological well-being, also contributes to cognitive resilience by reducing the negative impact of chronic stress on brain function. These findings suggest that personality dimensions can serve as predictors of cognitive aging trajectories and that fostering positive personality traits may enhance cognitive health in aging populations.The success of these interventions underscores the potential for implementing targeted cognitive enhancement programs to promote brain health in aging populations [
]. The SLS has demonstrated that cognitive training, when combined with lifestyle modifications such as physical activity and social engagement, can yield even greater benefits for cognitive resilience [ ]. Furthermore, the study's findings suggest that early intervention-beginning cognitive training before significant decline occurs-may be more effective in preserving cognitive function over time [ ]. These insights have informed the development of evidence-based cognitive training programs used in both clinical and community settings, highlighting the importance of proactive approaches to maintaining cognitive health in older adults. By integrating cognitive training with broader health promotion efforts, researchers and practitioners can work toward enhancing the quality of life for aging individuals and reducing the burden of cognitive decline on individuals and society.The SLS has also contributed to refining the understanding of when cognitive decline becomes reliably detectable. According to the study's longitudinal data, measurable decrements in cognitive performance typically begin in the mid-to-late sixties, although there is considerable variation among individuals [
]. Factors such as education, occupational complexity, and cognitive engagement appear to moderate the onset and severity of decline, with highly educated individuals and those engaged in mentally stimulating activities exhibiting delayed cognitive deterioration [ ]. These findings have important implications for early detection and intervention strategies aimed at preserving cognitive function in aging populations.Furthermore, the SLS has highlighted the significance of individual differences in cognitive aging. While some individuals experience only minimal cognitive decline, others show more rapid deterioration, underscoring the heterogeneity of aging trajectories [
]. This variability suggests that cognitive aging is not solely determined by biological factors but is also influenced by lifestyle, environmental exposures, and psychosocial variables. The study's emphasis on individual differences has paved the way for personalized approaches to cognitive assessment and intervention, reinforcing the idea that cognitive aging is a multifaceted process shaped by both intrinsic and extrinsic factors [ ].Additionally, the SLS has significantly advanced the understanding of cohort effects in cognitive aging, illustrating how historical and environmental factors shape cognitive development across different generations [
]. The study revealed that more recent cohorts tend to perform better on cognitive tests than earlier ones, a finding that aligns with the Flynn effect and highlights the role of societal advancements such as improved education, healthcare, and cognitive stimulation in shaping cognitive outcomes [ ]. These insights have led to revisions in existing aging theories, prompting researchers to incorporate generational influences when modeling cognitive change over the lifespan.Moreover, the SLS has contributed to the evolving concept of cognitive plasticity, demonstrating that cognitive abilities can be maintained or even enhanced through targeted interventions [
]. The study's findings on cognitive training programs and lifestyle modifications have challenged deterministic views of cognitive decline, supporting the notion that aging does not necessarily equate to inevitable cognitive deterioration. Instead, the SLS has reinforced the idea that cognitive aging is a dynamic process influenced by both biological and environmental factors, thereby encouraging the development of intervention-based approaches to promote cognitive resilience in older adults [ ].Another contentious issue concerns the efficacy of cognitive training interventions. Although the SLS has demonstrated that structured cognitive exercises can enhance cognitive abilities in older adults, some meta-analyses have questioned the generalizability and long-term benefits of such interventions [
]. Critics argue that while cognitive training may improve performance on specific tasks, the transfer of these gains to real-world cognitive functioning remains limited [ ]. Furthermore, methodological differences across studies-such as variations in training intensity, duration, and outcome measures-have led to inconsistencies in reported effects, complicating efforts to establish definitive conclusions about the utility of cognitive training as a broad intervention strategy [ ].Additionally, interpretations of cohort effects have varied among researchers. While the SLS attributes generational differences in cognitive performance primarily to improvements in education and healthcare, alternative explanations suggest that factors such as test familiarity, technological exposure, and changing norms in cognitive assessment may also contribute to observed trends [
]. These divergent perspectives highlight the complexity of cognitive aging research and underscore the need for continued investigation into the mechanisms underlying cognitive change across the lifespan.Another significant area of advancement lies in the exploration of biomarkers associated with cognitive aging. Researchers have identified various biological indicators-such as levels of amyloid-beta, tau proteins, and inflammatory markers-that correlate with cognitive decline and the risk of neurodegenerative diseases like Alzheimer's [
]. These biomarkers provide valuable tools for early detection and monitoring of cognitive changes, potentially enabling more timely interventions [ ]. Additionally, genetic studies have uncovered specific gene variants, such as APOE ε4, that increase susceptibility to cognitive decline, further elucidating the interplay between genetics and environmental factors in shaping cognitive aging trajectories [ ]. These findings complement the SLS's emphasis on individual differences and offer new avenues for personalized approaches to cognitive health.Moreover, recent research has expanded the scope of cognitive aging beyond traditional cognitive domains to include social cognition and emotional regulation. Studies have shown that older adults often exhibit preserved or even enhanced emotional intelligence and social problem-solving abilities, despite declines in fluid intelligence [
]. This nuanced perspective aligns with the SLS's recognition of cognitive diversity in aging and reinforces the importance of adopting a multidimensional approach to studying cognitive change [ ]. Collectively, these recent developments enrich our understanding of cognitive aging and pave the way for innovative interventions aimed at promoting cognitive resilience in later life.Another promising avenue for future research lies in investigating the role of modifiable lifestyle factors in promoting cognitive resilience (Seattle longitudinal study - UW department of psychiatry & behavioral sciences, n.d.) [
]. While the Seattle Longitudinal Study has established links between education, occupation, and cognitive outcomes, emerging research suggests that additional factors-such as diet, sleep quality, and mindfulness practices-may also play a crucial role in maintaining cognitive health [ ]. Longitudinal studies incorporating these variables could provide deeper insights into how lifestyle choices interact with biological and environmental determinants of cognitive aging. Additionally, experimental interventions targeting these lifestyle factors-such as dietary modifications, sleep hygiene programs, and mindfulness-based stress reduction-could yield valuable evidence on their efficacy in preventing or delaying cognitive decline.Furthermore, future research should prioritize cross-cultural comparisons to assess the generalizability of cognitive aging findings across diverse populations [
]. The Seattle Longitudinal Study, while groundbreaking, primarily focused on a Western demographic, limiting the applicability of its conclusions to other cultural contexts [ ]. Expanding cognitive aging research to include underrepresented populations will help address disparities in cognitive health outcomes and ensure that interventions are culturally appropriate and widely applicable. These directions underscore the evolving nature of cognitive aging research and highlight the need for interdisciplinary collaboration to advance our understanding of this complex phenomenon.Synthesizing the findings from the Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) and recent cognitive aging research reveals a cohesive yet evolving understanding of how cognitive abilities change across the lifespan (Schaie, 2005a) [
]. Both bodies of research affirm that cognitive aging is a heterogeneous process, marked by differential declines across cognitive domains rather than a uniform deterioration of intelligence [ ]. The SLS established foundational insights into the stability of verbal comprehension and crystallized intelligence well into later life, contrasting with the more pronounced decline observed in fluid intelligence components such as perceptual speed and inductive reasoning [ ]. Contemporary studies have corroborated these observations while expanding the scope of cognitive aging to include social cognition and emotional regulation, demonstrating that older adults often retain or even enhance certain cognitive-emotional competencies [ ].A key convergence between the SLS and recent research lies in the recognition of individual differences in cognitive aging trajectories. The SLS emphasized the role of education, occupation, and cognitive engagement in moderating the onset and severity of cognitive decline [
]. Recent studies have extended this understanding by identifying additional modifiable factors-such as diet, sleep quality, and mindfulness-that contribute to cognitive resilience [ ]. These findings collectively support the view that cognitive aging is not solely determined by biological inevitabilities but is significantly influenced by lifestyle and environmental exposures. Moreover, both the SLS and contemporary research underscore the importance of early intervention, suggesting that cognitive training and lifestyle modifications can delay or mitigate age-related cognitive decline [ ]. This synthesis highlights the dynamic nature of cognitive aging and reinforces the potential for targeted interventions to enhance cognitive health in aging populations.Despite the valuable contributions of the Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) and recent cognitive aging research, several limitations must be acknowledged to refine future investigations [
]. One major limitation of the SLS pertains to its sample composition, which was predominantly composed of middle-class, Caucasian individuals residing in the Seattle metropolitan area [ ]. This homogeneity restricts the generalizability of its findings to more diverse populations, particularly those from different cultural, socioeconomic, and ethnic backgrounds. While the study attempted to account for cohort effects by comparing successive generations, its geographic and demographic constraints limit the applicability of its conclusions to broader, global contexts [ ].Another limitation of the SLS relates to the reliance on standardized cognitive assessments, which, while reliable, may not fully capture the complexity of real-world cognitive functioning [
]. Traditional psychometric tests often emphasize domain-specific abilities such as memory, reasoning, and processing speed but may overlook more nuanced aspects of cognition, such as social intelligence, emotional regulation, and executive decision-making in naturalistic settings [ ]. Furthermore, the SLS's focus on cognitive decline primarily in later adulthood may underestimate the role of early-life factors, such as childhood cognitive development and educational experiences, in shaping long-term cognitive trajectories [ ].Contemporary cognitive aging research, while benefiting from advanced methodologies like neuroimaging and biomarker analysis, also faces challenges related to reproducibility and methodological consistency [
]. Variations in study designs, cognitive outcome measures, and statistical approaches make it difficult to compare findings across different research teams and populations. Additionally, while biomarker research has made significant strides in identifying early indicators of cognitive decline, translating these discoveries into practical, scalable interventions remains a challenge [ ]. Addressing these limitations will require greater interdisciplinary collaboration, standardized research protocols, and the inclusion of more diverse participant samples in future cognitive aging studies [ ].Building upon the findings and limitations of the Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) and recent cognitive aging research, several promising directions for future inquiry emerge [
]. One critical area involves expanding research to include more diverse populations in terms of ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and geographic location. Given that the SLS primarily focused on a homogeneous group of middle-class Caucasians in the Pacific Northwest, future studies should aim to replicate and extend these findings in underrepresented communities) [ ]. Incorporating cross-cultural comparisons will enhance the generalizability of cognitive aging research and provide insights into how sociocultural and environmental factors influence cognitive trajectories across different populations [ ].Another key direction involves integrating multimodal data collection methods to capture a more comprehensive picture of cognitive aging. While the SLS relied heavily on psychometric assessments and self-reported health metrics, modern research can benefit from combining cognitive testing with neuroimaging, genetic profiling, and biomarker analysis [
]. These integrative approaches can help identify early warning signs of cognitive decline, track disease progression, and evaluate the effectiveness of interventions at both behavioral and biological levels [ ]. Additionally, leveraging machine learning and artificial intelligence techniques can enhance predictive modeling of cognitive aging, allowing for more personalized risk assessments and targeted interventions [ ].Furthermore, future research should prioritize the development and evaluation of scalable cognitive interventions that can be implemented in real-world settings [
]. While the SLS demonstrated the potential of cognitive training to mitigate age-related decline, questions remain regarding the long-term sustainability and transferability of these benefits to everyday functioning [ ]. Investigating the efficacy of digital cognitive training platforms, lifestyle modification programs, and community-based interventions can provide practical solutions for promoting cognitive health across the lifespan. These directions collectively represent a forward-looking agenda for cognitive aging research, aiming to enhance both scientific understanding and public health outcomes.The Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) has profoundly shaped our understanding of cognitive aging, providing a robust empirical foundation for examining how cognitive abilities evolve across the lifespan [
]. Its longitudinal design, spanning multiple generations, has enabled researchers to disentangle the complex interplay of age, cohort, and period effects influencing cognitive development [ ]. By demonstrating that cognitive decline is neither uniform nor inevitable, the SLS has challenged outdated assumptions about aging and intelligence, paving the way for more nuanced theories of cognitive development [ ]. Notably, the study has highlighted the differential trajectories of fluid and crystallized intelligence, showing that while certain cognitive domains decline with age, others remain stable or even improve [ ]. These insights have been instrumental in informing both theoretical models and practical interventions aimed at preserving cognitive function in later life.Beyond its theoretical contributions, the SLS has had tangible implications for public policy, healthcare, and cognitive intervention strategies [
]. Its findings have been cited in legal discussions on age discrimination and retirement policies, reinforcing the argument that cognitive capabilities vary widely among older adults and should not be universally discounted [ ]. Moreover, the study's emphasis on modifiable factors such as education, occupational engagement, and lifestyle choices has underscored the potential for cognitive interventions to mitigate age-related decline [ ]. These findings continue to inform contemporary research, particularly in the development of cognitive training programs, digital health tools, and preventive strategies aimed at promoting cognitive resilience [ ]. As cognitive aging research continues to evolve, the legacy of the SLS remains a cornerstone in advancing our understanding of how to support cognitive health across the lifespan [ ].This work was supported in part by artificial intelligence tools, including Qwen (Tongyi Lab), to assist with structuring, formatting, and refining content. All final content, interpretations, and conclusions were reviewed and approved by the author. The use of AI did not influence the design, analysis, or core arguments presented in this manuscript.
The author would like to express their sincere gratitude to the following individuals and institutions for their support, whether directly or indirectly, during this research.
I gratefully acknowledge the intellectual environment and academic resources provided by K. International School Tokyo, which greatly contributed to the development and completion of this work.
Special thanks are extended to John Rose and Thomas Waterfall, whose insights and suggestions were invaluable during the early stages of conceptualization, even though the subsequent development and execution of the research were carried out independently.
I also wish to thank the anonymous reviewers whose feedback helped strengthen the clarity and rigor of the final manuscript.
Lastly, I acknowledge the support of family and friends, whose encouragement and patience were instrumental in maintaining focus and motivation throughout this project.
Chak Hang Chan conducted the literature search, selected and synthesized the sources, drafted and revised the manuscript, and approved the final version for submission. The author is solely responsible for the content and accuracy of this narrative review.
About SLS - Seattle Longitudinal Study. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences [Internet].
Schaie KW, Willis SL, Caskie GIL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition [Internet]. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences; 2020 Mar.
Seattle Longitudinal Study - UW Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences [Internet].
Hertzog C. Methodological issues in the assessment of fluid and crystallized intelligence in aging research. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 6th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2010;117–34.
Schaie KW. Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press; 1996.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
Schaie KW. Impact of life-course factors on patterns of cognitive aging. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000;217–32.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.
Prinzie P, Onghena P. Modeling developmental change: Applications of multilevel modeling to longitudinal data. In: Laursen B, Little TD, Card NA, editors. Modeling Longitudinal and Multilevel Data. New York: Routledge; 2005;11–28.
Nordlund A, Påhlsson B, Holmberg H, Lind K, Wallin J. Cognitive aging: Progress in understanding and opportunities for action. Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2011.
Kennison RF, Situ D, Reyes N, Ahacic K. Cohort effects. In: Pachana NA, editor. Encyclopedia of Geropsychology. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2016;1–9.
Schaie KW, Willis SL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study of adult cognitive development. NIH Public Access [Internet]. 2010
Schaie KW, Willis SL, Caskie GIL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition [Internet]. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences; 2020.
Schaie KW. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: a thirty-five-year inquiry of adult intellectual development. Z Gerontol. 1993 May-Jun;26(3):129-37. PMID: 8337905.
Abramson A. Psychology is improving brain health and aging. Am Psychol Assoc [Internet]. 2024.
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Cognitive aging: Progress in understanding and opportunities for action. Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2015.
Creativity and intelligence: A tripartite structure. Psychology Today. 2012.
Crystallized intelligence - The Behavioral Scientist. The Behavioral Scientist [Internet]. [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Hueluer G, Dodge H. New developments in cognitive aging research. Innov Aging. 2018;2(Suppl 1):382.
Khalil R, Godde B, Karim AA. The Link Between Creativity, Cognition, and Creative Drives and Underlying Neural Mechanisms. Front Neural Circuits. 2019 Mar 22;13:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00018. PMID: 30967763; PMCID: PMC6440443.
Maelstrom Research. Maelstrom research - Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) [Internet]. Maelstrom Research; [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Qwen3-235B-A22B (Apr 29 version) [Large language model]. 2025.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
University of Cambridge. Study finds strongest evidence to date of brain's ability to compensate for age-related cognitive decline. ScienceDaily [Internet]. 2024 Jan 15 [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Schaie KW, Willis SL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study of Adult Cognitive Development. ISSBD Bull. 2010;57(1):24-29. PMID: 23536918; PMCID: PMC3607395.
McKnight Brain Research Foundation. What is cognitive aging? [Internet]. 2021 Apr [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. What is cognitive reserve? [Internet]. 2023 Jan 17.
Yu C, Beckmann JF, Birney DP. Cognitive flexibility as a meta-competency / Flexibilidad cognitiva como meta-competencia. Estud Psicol (Campinas). 2019;40(3):563–84.
Zhao S, Li Y, Shi Y, Li X. Cognitive Aging: How the Brain Ages? Adv Exp Med Biol. 2023;1419:9-21. doi: 10.1007/978-981-99-1627-6_2. PMID: 37418203.
Chan CH. Unraveling Cognitive Aging: A Comprehensive Narrative Review of the Seattle Longitudinal Study and Recent Breakthroughs. IgMin Res. August 21, 2025; 3(8): 315-323. IgMin ID: igmin312; DOI:10.61927/igmin312; Available at: igmin.link/p312
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
K. International School Tokyo, Japan
Address Correspondence:
Chak Hang Chan, K. International School Tokyo, Japan, Email: [email protected]
How to cite this article:
Chan CH. Unraveling Cognitive Aging: A Comprehensive Narrative Review of the Seattle Longitudinal Study and Recent Breakthroughs. IgMin Res. August 21, 2025; 3(8): 315-323. IgMin ID: igmin312; DOI:10.61927/igmin312; Available at: igmin.link/p312
Copyright: © 2025 Chan CH This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
About SLS - Seattle Longitudinal Study. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences [Internet].
Schaie KW, Willis SL, Caskie GIL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition [Internet]. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences; 2020 Mar.
Seattle Longitudinal Study - UW Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences [Internet].
Hertzog C. Methodological issues in the assessment of fluid and crystallized intelligence in aging research. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 6th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2010;117–34.
Schaie KW. Intellectual Development in Adulthood: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press; 1996.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
Schaie KW. Impact of life-course factors on patterns of cognitive aging. In: Birren JE, Schaie KW, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000;217–32.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.
Prinzie P, Onghena P. Modeling developmental change: Applications of multilevel modeling to longitudinal data. In: Laursen B, Little TD, Card NA, editors. Modeling Longitudinal and Multilevel Data. New York: Routledge; 2005;11–28.
Nordlund A, Påhlsson B, Holmberg H, Lind K, Wallin J. Cognitive aging: Progress in understanding and opportunities for action. Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2011.
Kennison RF, Situ D, Reyes N, Ahacic K. Cohort effects. In: Pachana NA, editor. Encyclopedia of Geropsychology. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2016;1–9.
Schaie KW, Willis SL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study of adult cognitive development. NIH Public Access [Internet]. 2010
Schaie KW, Willis SL, Caskie GIL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition [Internet]. University of Washington Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences; 2020.
Schaie KW. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: a thirty-five-year inquiry of adult intellectual development. Z Gerontol. 1993 May-Jun;26(3):129-37. PMID: 8337905.
Abramson A. Psychology is improving brain health and aging. Am Psychol Assoc [Internet]. 2024.
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Cognitive aging: Progress in understanding and opportunities for action. Washington (DC): National Academies Press; 2015.
Creativity and intelligence: A tripartite structure. Psychology Today. 2012.
Crystallized intelligence - The Behavioral Scientist. The Behavioral Scientist [Internet]. [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Hueluer G, Dodge H. New developments in cognitive aging research. Innov Aging. 2018;2(Suppl 1):382.
Khalil R, Godde B, Karim AA. The Link Between Creativity, Cognition, and Creative Drives and Underlying Neural Mechanisms. Front Neural Circuits. 2019 Mar 22;13:18. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00018. PMID: 30967763; PMCID: PMC6440443.
Maelstrom Research. Maelstrom research - Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS) [Internet]. Maelstrom Research; [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Qwen3-235B-A22B (Apr 29 version) [Large language model]. 2025.
Schaie KW. Developmental Influences on Adult Intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013.
University of Cambridge. Study finds strongest evidence to date of brain's ability to compensate for age-related cognitive decline. ScienceDaily [Internet]. 2024 Jan 15 [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Schaie KW, Willis SL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study of Adult Cognitive Development. ISSBD Bull. 2010;57(1):24-29. PMID: 23536918; PMCID: PMC3607395.
McKnight Brain Research Foundation. What is cognitive aging? [Internet]. 2021 Apr [cited 2025 Apr 5].
Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. What is cognitive reserve? [Internet]. 2023 Jan 17.
Yu C, Beckmann JF, Birney DP. Cognitive flexibility as a meta-competency / Flexibilidad cognitiva como meta-competencia. Estud Psicol (Campinas). 2019;40(3):563–84.
Zhao S, Li Y, Shi Y, Li X. Cognitive Aging: How the Brain Ages? Adv Exp Med Biol. 2023;1419:9-21. doi: 10.1007/978-981-99-1627-6_2. PMID: 37418203.